Monday, March 20, 2006
Friday, March 17, 2006
Oh no, I'm going to get in trouble!

Berthoz and her colleagues had subjects read short stories describing transgressions of social rules. The stories either described situations where (1) the subject was the agent of transgression, and the violations was accidental, or situations where (2) the protagonist was another person than the subject, and the violations was accidental, or situations where (3) the subject was the agent of transgression, and the violations was intentional, or situations where (4) the protagonist was another person than the subject, and the violation was intentional. Thus, Berthoz et al. were able to contrast intentional moral transgressions performed by one self from transgressions performed by others, or from accidental transgressions.
This contrast showed significant bilateral amygdala activation, and Berthoz et al. speculate that such activation may be related to one's anticipation of possible punishment as a consequence of one's own immoral behaviour. This suggestion, of course, squares well with ideas from the emerging field of social neuroscience - especially the hypothesis that social cooperation rests upon a tit-for-tat regime: If I share my ressources with you, I expect something in return. If I don't get anything back, I will punish you. It is pretty clear, as well, that the back-bone of the success of such social behaviour is the brain's reward and punishment system: The expectancy of a return is modulated by the reward system, and the anticipation of a punishment - which works to keep you from cheating the other members of your social group - is modulated by the punishment system, including the amygdala.
Now, it would be very interesting to apply a genetic analysis to this result. Maybe we would then find a similar variance as reported by Hariri with regard to serotonin re-uptake and mood? That is, some people may be more afraid of transgressing moral rules than others due to a difference in amygdala activity. It is rather obvious, after all, that some people won't loose any sleep over sticking it to you!
Reference
Berthoz, S. et al. (2006): Affective response to one's own moral violations. To appear in NeuroImage.
Thursday, March 16, 2006
Hariri review in TICS

Reference
Hariri, A. & Holmes, A. (2006): Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. To appear in Trends in Cognitive Science.
Wednesday, March 15, 2006
Genes, brain/mind and behaviour
7th EMBL/EMBO Joint Conference 2006
3-4 November 2006, EMBL Heidelberg, Germany
Genes, brain/mind and behaviour
Research in the life sciences is revealing how genes are differentially expressed in the brain and how types of behavior reflect the functioning of different neural networks. Scientists are also exploring the relationship between the neurophysiology of the brain and the nature of consciousness.
Science and technology always work in tandem. Neurotechnology refers to the set of tools that have been developed to analyze and influence the human nervous system, especially the brain. We would like to assess the uses that are – or could in the future be - made of new neurological knowledge and technologies. What are the consequences when biochemical solutions to behavioral problems such as depression, addiction, or eating disorders take precedence over attempts to repair the social environment, or defective inter-personal relations? How do we avert the risk of psychopharmacology being abused for neurochemical enhancement?
While new knowledge coming out of the neurosciences has an enormous potential for beneficial applications in diverse fields, treating or manipulating the mind will also have important social, legal and bioethical implications. These are some of the main issues that will be the focus of the next inter-disciplinary EMBL/EMBO Science and Society conference in 2006 in Heidelberg, Germany.
Programme
Poster [2MB]
Normative brains and group studies

Human brains differ in size and shape, and one goal of spatial normalization is to deform human brain scans so one location in one subject's brain scan corresponds to the same location in another subject's brain scan.Human brains are like fingerprints. On a general level they are alike, but they differ significantly when we look at the details. There are large individual differences if we look at where sulci and gyri appear and disappear in the brain; even whether you have one or two sulci.
Krishnan et al. have looked at the hippocampus, and found large variations in the position and extent of spatial normalization, not only due to sample size (i.e. how many subjects were included), but also when comparing the operation in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment and healthy subjects. Put another way, if you do spatial normalization -- which you do if you want to compare groups at the brain level -- the operation may induce a bias in your results. For example, it may lead you to think that the loss of hippocampal gray matter is larger than it actually is.
To open up the lid slightly to one of my forthcoming publications, we have looked at more structures of the medial temporal lobe, including the temporopolar, entorhinal, perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex, as well as the amygdala and hippocampus. We have found that spatial normalization of these areas leads to significant dicplacement of the different structures. It's sometimes so bad that what is identified as the perirhinal cortex in the original brain (native space) is partly displaced into the hippocampus.
Here is an image of the coregistration pandemonium in the medial temporal lobe. It shows the coregistration of the left perirhinal cortex in six subjects. The structure was drawn as a region of interest and then normalized according to standard SPM warping:

This has a tremendous impact both theoretically and clinically. There is currently a huge interest in this region and whether it is specifically operation in memory (the Squire-Zola model), or whether it has additional roles in visual perception, novelty processing and cross-modal perception (see latest article by Buckley & Gaffan).
Think of it: if you go through all papers reporting hippocampal activation in an fMRI paradigm, but if you re-do the analysis properly, or look at the individual scans, you see that most of the hippocampal activation is actually perirhinal. I'll never trust a spatially normalized image again.
OK, to some people this is old news, but let's face it: most of us eat the results from group studies raw, without chewing too much about how these images were made, i.e. normalized. Well, now I hope you do.
If you want to read more I have two relevant abstracts:
Monday, March 13, 2006
How genes make up your mind
Just a small passage from my piece:
"Genes control the development of neurons to make up brains, but they also govern neuronal gene expression during our daily lives. (...) Genes work at every level of the neural process. They are the fundamental building blocks for both the structure and the functioning of the brain. They set the stage for how neurons and functional groups of neurons act in response to different inputs. Genes are therefore fundamental for the way we experience, think and behave."
Tuesday, March 07, 2006
Im-Gen videos now up

As I mentioned in this post, the videos from this year's International Imaging Genetics Conference would be just around the corner. Turns out that was a precise prediction. The videos for all speakers are now online. Buy some chips and a cola and put yourself in front of a double-screen projector (one with the video and one with the PDF) and enjoy!
Friday, March 03, 2006
CIMBI alive
One part of the CIMBI project involves looking at how genes coding for seretonin affect the seretonin transport function, and furthermore how the function of seretonergic areas of the brain operate depending on the genetic makeup of a subject. In this latter part, I am involved in doing the MRI study, including three fMRI protocols:
- Processing of facial affect - how genes affect the processing of facial expression, especially the difference between aversive and neutral faces.
- Memory processing in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) - how different parts of the MTL make different contribution to specific phases in memory processing: preparation, encoding, rehearsal and retrieval.
- Categorization task - the difference between choosing between high-specificity options (within-category choices, e.g. "donkey or zebra") or low-specificity options (between-category choices, e.g. "living or non-living")
Imaging Genetics stuff
I have a few comments to some of the presentations, and to keep in mind:
- Nik Schork presented and discussed ways to visualize the imaging/genetics data (especially the latter. Unfortunately, these slides are still missing from the list
- Tom Nichols gave a good talk that to me helped binding the fields of genetics, neuroimaging and statistics a bit more together. Again, these slides are also missing
- Bernie Devlin is worth having in mind when thinking critically about the projects done in this field.
- Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg gave a really cool talk about his work on Williams-Beuren syndrome, and how genes play a role in the brain in social cognition.
- Ahmad Hariri gave an equally neat talk about individual differences in appetitive drive.
- Dan Weinberger has this extra way of grabbing his audience's attention and make them forget all about jet lag and coffee thirst. His talk about the lessons from the study of COMT were most interesting.